Skip to main content
European Commission logo
English English
CORDIS - EU research results
CORDIS
CORDIS Web 30th anniversary CORDIS Web 30th anniversary

Article Category

Content archived on 2023-03-06

Article available in the following languages:

How the brain links memory and behaviour

A research team has identified which parts of the brain are responsible for different types of memory, a result that could have major implications for our understanding of neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia. The study, 'From rapid place learning to behavioural ...

A research team has identified which parts of the brain are responsible for different types of memory, a result that could have major implications for our understanding of neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia. The study, 'From rapid place learning to behavioural performance: A key role for the intermediate hippocampus', published online in PLoS Biology, was led by Dr Tobias Bast of the UK's University of Nottingham. The researchers set out to define which parts of the brain are responsible for which types of memory. For example, a repetitive task such as driving to work requires a different type of memory than standing at the fridge trying to remember what you wanted to take out or wondering where you put your keys. The research team focused their investigations on the hippocampus, a banana shaped part of the brain beneath the temporal lobe. The hippocampus is responsible for 'place cell firing' - the ability to hone memory to certain places. Previous hippocampal research on rats has shown that hippocampal neurons are fired when the rat passes a certain place, sparking its memory. But until now, how this hippocampal memory is translated into behaviour, has not received great attention. Dr Bast's team tested rats in a water maze experiment, in which the animals had to locate a platform in the water. Different parts of the rats' hippocampus were disabled using a neurotoxin and their consequent memory abilities were measured. The results showed that if approximately 30-40% of neuronal tissue in the middle of the rats' hippocampus - called the intermediate region - was left intact, the rats could still carry out the task of identifying where the water platform was. But when the intermediate hippocampus was disabled, with only 30-40% of tissue at two ends of the hippocampus (the 'septal' and 'temporal' regions) left intact, the rats struggled with the task. The research also revealed that the septal end of the hippocampus, which has links to precise visual-spatial information, can still quickly form an accurate place memory, but cannot translate this into behaviour because it needs the help of the intermediate hippocampus to provide links to behavioural control. 'People often focus on memory deficits when thinking about the significance of aberrant hippocampal function,' said Dr Bast. 'But our new findings highlight the important hippocampal links to behavioural control. We plan to build on these findings and examine the possibility that aberrant hippocampal function - depending on where in the structure it occurs and to which extent - may give rise to selective memory deficits, as well as to more profound disruptions of behavioural control.' The team plans to continue its research by focusing on how abnormal hippocampal activity is involved in many neuropsychiatric conditions such as schizophrenia.

Countries

United Kingdom

Related articles